Original Paper
Abstract
Background: Promoting vaccination and eliminating vaccine hesitancy are key measures for controlling vaccine-preventable diseases.
Objective: We aimed to understand the beliefs surrounding and drivers of vaccination behavior, and their relationships with and influence on vaccination intention and practices.
Methods: We conducted a web-based survey in 31 provinces in mainland China from May 24, 2021 to June 15, 2021, with questions pertaining to vaccination in 5 dimensions: attitude, subjective norms, perceived behavioral control, intention, and behavior. We performed hierarchical regression analysis and structural equation modeling based on the theory of planned behavior—in which, the variables attitude, subjective norms, and intention each affect the variable intention; the variable intention mediates the relationships of attitude and subjective norms with behavior, and the variable perceived behavioral control moderates the strength of this mediation—to test the validity of the theoretical framework.
Results: A total of 9924 participants, aged 18 to 59 years, were included in this study. Vaccination intention mediated the relationships of attitude and subjective norms with vaccination behavior. The indirect effect of attitude on vaccination behavior was 0.164 and that of subjective norms was 0.255, and the difference was statistically significant (P<.001). The moderated mediation analysis further indicated that perceived behavioral control would affect the mediation when used as moderator, and the interaction terms for attitude (β=–0.052, P<.001) and subjective norms (β=–0.028, P=.006) with perceived behavioral control were significant.
Conclusions: Subjective norms have stronger positive influences on vaccination practices than attitudes. Perceived behavioral control, as a moderator, has a substitution relationship with attitudes and subjective norms and weakens their positive effects on vaccination behavior.
doi:10.2196/34666
Keywords
Introduction
Vaccines play a crucial role in protection against infectious diseases. Vaccination is an important component of both family and public health. However, confusion and misunderstanding still surround vaccines, even though they protect against a wide variety of organisms that cause disease, such as influenza, cervical cancer, hepatitis B, COVID-19, pneumonia, and rabies. A recent study [
] by the COVID-19 reaction team at Imperial College London, found that high vaccine hesitancy rates can significantly prolong the time required for nondrug interventions to maintain and decrease the mortality associated with COVID-19. Therefore, exploring the mechanisms behind vaccination behavior to reduce vaccine hesitancy and improve vaccination rates has become a key research topic. Research on vaccine acceptance suggests that individual decisions about vaccination behavior are much more complex and may involve emotional, cultural, social, spiritual, or political factors, as well as cognitive factors [ , ]. It has been demonstrated that theory-based behavioral interventions are more effective [ - ]. However, rather than building on the premise of theoretical models to test hypotheses, most studies [ - ] that have collected data to conduct exploratory studies of knowledge, attitudes, and beliefs or have focused on demographic factors related to vaccination practices.Currently, the theory of planned behavior, which is one of the most commonly used psychological theories to explain health behavior, is considered to be the most suitable for explaining vaccination behavior [
, - ], and has been used effectively as a theoretical framework for designing health behavior interventions [ , ]. Another framework—the health belief model—is also widely used in behavioral health fields; however, studies [ - ] have shown that the health belief model is more suited to description rather than explanation of health behavior and has weak predictive validity. Although Webb et al [ ] found that theory of planned behavior–based interventions were more effective than those based on other theories, existing studies on theory of planned behavior have not been in-depth—a systematic review [ ] demonstrated that most researchers did not address adaptive feedback, merely focused on intention as the outcome, and ignored mediating effects between intentions and behaviors. Yet, the purpose of the theory of planned behavior was to account for perceived behavioral control, which, as a representative of actual behavioral control, should have an impact on the overall model [ ].The beliefs and behavioral drivers of vaccination need to be studied to be able to develop better targeted intervention strategies. We aimed to confirm the theoretical validity and ability of the theory of planned behavior to explain vaccination practices.
Methods
Ethics Approval
The Wuxi Center for Disease Control and Prevention Ethics Committee approved this study (2020No10).
Participants and Procedure
From May 24 to June 15, 2021, we conducted surveys in 31 provinces in mainland China using web-based questionnaires. We used convenience sampling. The link to the questionnaire was created through Wen Juan Xing, which is a platform dedicated to the creation and dissemination of questionnaires, and forwarded by the WeChat platform of the Jiangsu Provincial Center for Disease Control and Prevention. Written information was provided as a statement that could be read, which assured participants that the study was conducted on a voluntary basis and for research purposes only. All surveys were conducted in Chinese. (The surveys were translated into English by 2 researchers only for the purpose of this paper.) Questionnaires answered by people under 18 years and over 60 years were not analyzed. To prevent repeated submission of questionnaires, WeChat real name verification was required when using the link to fill out the questionnaire, and an IP address could only be used to submit a questionnaire once. Questionnaires completed in less than 60 seconds were automatically discarded. Questionnaires with selections at the same level of the Likert scale were also considered invalid.
The questionnaire (
) was used to collect demographic information (age, gender, ethnicity, usual place of residence, education level, annual household income, and whether respondents worked in the health care industry, residential status), chronic disease history, and information on self-assessment of health status.Theoretical Background
The theory of planned behavior is widely used to study intentions and behaviors. In this model, intentions are considered the most direct predictor of behaviors and are weighted based on attitudes, which is the degree to which behaviors are positively or negatively evaluated, and subjective norms, which is the pressure society places on implementing or not implementing behaviors. The effects of attitudes and subjective norms are mediated by perceived behavioral control, which is people's perceptions of their ability to perform a given behavior. When perceived behavioral control is accurate, it acts as a proxy for actual behavioral control, that is, the extent to which a person has the ability, resources, and other conditions required to perform the behavior.
In this study, the variable attitude represented people's positive or negative perceptions of the vaccine. The variable subjective norms referred to the expectations of family, friends, and physicians. The variable perceived behavioral control represented people's beliefs about barriers to vaccination (such as the time, cost, and side effects caused by vaccination). Each item was assessed using a 5-point Likert scale (
).Dimension and questions | Scale | |
Attitude | 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree) | |
Q1. I think that vaccination is safe. | ||
Q2. I think that vaccination is effective. | ||
Q3. I think that vaccination is beneficial. | ||
Q4. I think that vaccination is important. | ||
Subjective norms | 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree) | |
Q5. Did my family, doctors, and close friends think I should be vaccinated? | ||
Q6. Will I do what they think I should do? | ||
Q7. Can vaccination can protect close relatives from relevant vaccine-preventable diseases? | ||
Perceived behavior control | 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree) | |
Q8. The possibility of still being infected after vaccination would discourage me from getting vaccinated. | ||
Q9. The exorbitant cost of vaccinating would stop me from getting vaccinated. | ||
Q10. Vaccination causes a decline in autoimmunity. | ||
Q11. Concerns about side effects of the vaccine stop me from getting vaccinated. | ||
Q12. Difficulty in obtaining an appointment for vaccination would prevent me from getting vaccinated. | ||
Intention | 1 (completely impossible) to 5 (completely possible) | |
Q13. The possibility of considering getting vaccinated. | ||
Q14. The possibility of trying to get vaccinated. | ||
Q15. The possibility of actually getting vaccinated. | ||
Behavior | 1 (completely impossible) to 5 (completely possible) | |
Q16. How likely are you to go for a COVID-19 vaccine? | ||
Q17. What is the possibility of getting an influenza shot this year? | ||
Q18. What is the level of hesitation about vaccinating? |
Hypotheses
Hypothesis 1
Attitude will have a positive association with vaccination behavior.
Hypothesis 2
Subjective norms will have a positive association with vaccination behavior.
Hypothesis 3
Vaccination intention will have a positive association with vaccination behavior.
Hypothesis 4
Vaccination intention will mediate the relationships of attitude (hypothesis 4a) and subjective norms (hypothesis 4b) with vaccination behavior.
Hypothesis 5
Perceived behavior control will moderate the strength of the mediated relationships of attitude (hypothesis 5a) and subjective norms (hypothesis 5b) with vaccination behavior via vaccination intention.
Model Testing
Before structural equation modeling, confirmatory factor analysis was conducted to assess the reliability and validity of the constructs. Reliability was assessed by calculating the squared multiple correlation [
] and composite reliability [ ]. We also examined parameter estimates and their associated t values, factor loadings, and the average variance extracted [ ]. We established discriminant validity by calculating the square root of the average variance extracted for each latent variable. The error variances and modification indices of items were estimated.Model Fitting
The goodness-of-fit index (GFI), adjusted GFI, comparative fit index (CFI), and the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) were used to evaluate the model fit [
]. For GFI, CFI, and adjusted GFI, values closer to 1 are better, and values greater than 0.95 indicate relatively good fit; RMSEA values less than 0.06 indicate relatively good fit [ , ].Statistical Analysis
We used bootstrapping (5000 trials) to test mediator effects [
]. Hierarchical moderator regression was used to test moderation effects, and all variables were standardized to avoid multicollinearity [ ]. The control variables were entered in the block 1 (gender, education level, health care occupation, annual household income, main living condition, self-evaluation of health, chronic diseases, past behavior), followed by the standardized value of the main effect (attitude, subjective norms, and perceived behavioral control) in block 2, and finally, the interactions and moderators (subjective norms * perceived behavioral control, attitude * perceived behavioral control) in block 3.AMOS software (version 23; IBM Corp) was used to estimate the structural equation coefficients between latent variables in the model. Hierarchical moderator regression analysis was performed using SPSS software (version 23; IBM Corp). Statistical significance was set at P<.05.
Results
Participant Information
A total of 9924 participants (male: 5407/9924, 54.5%; female: 4517/9924, 45.5%) were included in this study (
). Most respondents had a college degree or higher (7589/9924, 76.4%), and did not work in health care–related industries (7007/9924, 70.6%).In this study, the proportion showing vaccine hesitancy (respondents who selected not sure, hesitant, and very hesitant for Q18) was estimated to be about 26.6% (2640/9924). Of the total sample, 77% (7643/9924) reported that they had received COVID-19 vaccinations, and 29.4% (2922/9924) had received the influenza vaccinations in the previous year. Of the women, 22% (992/4517) had received human papillomavirus (HPV) vaccinations. Of the total sample, of the majority believed that they would choose to receive COVID-19 (8614/9924, 86.8%) and influenza (3315/9924, 33.4%), vaccinations this year.
Characteristic | Respondents (n=9924), n (%) | |
Age group (years) | ||
18-24 | 2362 (23.8) | |
25-34 | 3963 (39.9) | |
35-44 | 2334 (23.5) | |
45-54 | 1125 (11.3) | |
55-59 | 140 (1.4) | |
Gender | ||
Male | 5407 (54.5) | |
Female | 4517 (45.5) | |
Educational level | ||
High school graduate or below | 2335 (23.5) | |
College or equivalent | 6822 (68.7) | |
Master’s diploma or above | 767 (7.7) | |
Health care occupation | ||
Yes | 2917 (29.4) | |
No | 7007 (70.6) | |
Annual household income (US $) | ||
<16,000 | 4285 (43.2) | |
16,000-32,000 | 4259 (42.9) | |
32,000-80,000 | 1112 (11.2) | |
>80,000 | 268 (2.7) | |
Main living condition | ||
Living with others | 9145 (92.2) | |
Alone | 779 (7.8) | |
Self-evaluation of health | ||
Very bad | 359 (3.6) | |
Bad | 242 (2.4) | |
General | 3168 (31.9) | |
Well | 3816 (38.5) | |
Very well | 2339 (23.6) | |
Chronic diseases | ||
Yes | 1275 (12.8) | |
No | 8649 (87.2) | |
Influenza vaccination history (last year) | ||
Yes | 2922 (29.4) | |
No | 7002 (70.6) | |
COVID-19 vaccination history | ||
Yes | 7643 (77.0) | |
No | 2281 (23.0) | |
HPVa vaccination history (n=4517) | ||
Yes | 992 (22.0) | |
No | 3525 (78.0) |
aHPV: human papillomavirus.
Measurement Model and Fitting
The 18 items were found to be reliable and valid based on each item’s estimated error variance and modification index (Table S1 and Table S2 in
). Composite reliability values were greater than 0.6 and average variance extracted values were greater than 0.5, except those for the dimension behavior ( ).The square roots of average variance extracted of the dimensions attitude, subjective norms, intention, and perceived behavioral control exceeded the related correlations (
), indicating discriminant validity in the structures in this study [ ]. The overall model achieved a good fit (GFI 0.991; CFI 0.992; adjusted GFI 0.987; RMSEA 0.029); therefore, the measurements and structural model were acceptable.Dimension | Parameter significance estimation | Factor loading | Squared multiple correlation | Composite reliability | Average variance extracted | ||||||||
Unstandardized estimate | SE | t value | P value | Standardized estimate | |||||||||
Attitude | 0.902 | 0.698 | |||||||||||
Q1 | 1.000 | 0.831 | 0.691 | ||||||||||
Q2 | 1.020 | 0.011 | 96.496 | <.001 | 0.837 | 0.701 | |||||||
Q3 | 0.968 | 0.010 | 96.319 | <.001 | 0.836 | 0.699 | |||||||
Q4 | 0.988 | 0.010 | 96.319 | <.001 | 0.836 | 0.699 | |||||||
Subjective norms | 0.759 | 0.514 | |||||||||||
Q5 | 1.000 | 0.628 | 0.394 | ||||||||||
Q6 | 1.104 | 0.022 | 50.029 | <.001 | 0.750 | 0.563 | |||||||
Q7 | 1.135 | 0.023 | 49.800 | <.001 | 0.765 | 0.585 | |||||||
Perceived behavioral control | 0.910 | 0.671 | |||||||||||
Q8 | 1.000 | 0.814 | 0.663 | ||||||||||
Q9 | 0.976 | 0.012 | 84.038 | <.001 | 0.761 | 0.579 | |||||||
Q10 | 1.008 | 0.010 | 99.196 | <.001 | 0.860 | 0.740 | |||||||
Q11 | 1.017 | 0.011 | 94.843 | <.001 | 0.832 | 0.692 | |||||||
Q12 | 0.996 | 0.011 | 93.580 | <.001 | 0.824 | 0.679 | |||||||
Intention | 0.863 | 0.678 | |||||||||||
Q13 | 1.000 | 0.811 | 0.658 | ||||||||||
Q14 | 1.028 | 0.012 | 84.088 | <.001 | 0.837 | 0.701 | |||||||
Q15 | 0.979 | 0.012 | 83.315 | <.001 | 0.821 | 0.674 | |||||||
Behavior | 0.549 | 0.300 | |||||||||||
Q16 | 1.000 | 0.434 | 0.188 | ||||||||||
Q17 | 2.062 | 0.083 | 24.826 | <.001 | 0.453 | 0.205 | |||||||
Q18 | 3.069 | 0.156 | 19.664 | <.001 | 0.712 | 0.507 |
Dimension | Average variance extracted | Dimension, correlation | ||||
Perceived behavioral control | Behavior | Intention | Subjective norms | Attitude | ||
Perceived behavioral control | 0.671 | 0.819 | —a | — | — | — |
Behavior | 0.300 | 0.618 | 0.548 | — | — | — |
Intention | 0.678 | 0.419 | 0.671 | 0.823 | — | — |
Subjective norms | 0.514 | 0.406 | 0.601 | 0.683 | 0.717 | — |
Attitude | 0.698 | 0.486 | 0.639 | 0.587 | 0.551 | 0.835 |
aRepeated correlation coefficients are omitted. The first occurrence value in each column of dimension is the square root of the average variance extracted for each latent variable.
Hierarchical Moderator Regression Analysis
Annual household income, education, gender, health care occupation, chronic diseases, health self-assessment, and past vaccination behavior affected vaccination behavior (∆R2=0.121, P<.001); however, whether participants lived alone or not did not significantly affect vaccination behavior as a control variable (P=.08) (Table S3 in
). In block 3, the attitude * perceived behavioral control and subjective norms * perceived behavioral control terms significantly changed the model compared with block 2 (∆R2=0.003, P<.001).In block 3, perceived behavioral control was positively and significantly correlated with vaccination behavior (β= 0.274, P<.001); however, attitude * perceived behavioral control (β=–0.052, P<.001) and subjective norms * perceived behavioral control (β=–0.028, P=.006) had reverse inhibitory effects on vaccination behavior. This not only indicates that perceived behavioral control moderates the impact of attitude on behavior and that of subjective norms on behavior, supporting hypotheses 5a and 5b, it also shows that there is a substitution relationship between attitude, subjective norms, and perceived behavioral control in their influence on vaccination behavior. Overall, perceived behavioral control weakens the positive effects of attitude and subjective norms on vaccination behavior, and when perceived behavioral control is low, the promotion effects of attitude and subjective norms on behavior are more pronounced, but with increases in perceived behavioral control, the positive effects of attitude and subjective norms on behavior gradually decrease. Specifically, the slope describing effect of attitude on behavior will decrease by 0.050 SD and that of subjective norms on behavior will decrease by 0.021 SD when perceived behavioral control increases by 1 SD (
; Table S3 in ).Model | △R2 | F value | P value |
Block 1 | 0.121 | 151.760 | <.001 |
Block 2 | 0.233 | 1192.784 | <.001 |
Block 3 | 0.003 | 23.348 | <.001 |
Structural Equation Model of Vaccination Behavior
Both attitude (direct effect: β=0.493, P<.001) and subjective norms (direct effect: β=0.244, P<.001) showed significant positive associations with behavior. Hence, hypotheses 1 and 2 were confirmed. Intention also positively and significantly affected vaccination behavior (direct effect: β=0.462, P<.001); therefore, hypothesis 3 was confirmed. In addition, bootstrapping indicated that intention was present as a positive and significant mediator between attitude and vaccination behavior (indirect effect: β=0.159, P<.001). Similarly, the mediating effect between subjective norms and vaccination behavior was positive and significant (indirect effect β=0.258, P<.001); therefore, hypotheses 4a and 4b were confirmed (
). Furthermore, the difference between the indirect effect of attitude on behavior and that of subjective norms on behavior was statistically significant (difference=–0.091, P<.001), which indicated that subjective norms had a greater influence on vaccination behavior than attitude.Effects | Unstandardized point estimate | SE | Bootstrapping | Bootstrapping, bias-corrected | P value | |||||
95% CI lower | 95% CI upper | 95% CI lower | 95% CI upper | |||||||
Direct | ||||||||||
Attitude---Intention | 0.344 | 0.021 | 0.303 | 0.384 | 0.303 | 0.384 | <.001 | |||
Subjective norms---Intention | 0.558 | 0.025 | 0.510 | 0.609 | 0.510 | 0.609 | <.001 | |||
Intention---Behavior | 0.462 | 0.030 | 0.404 | 0.522 | 0.403 | 0.521 | <.001 | |||
Attitude---Behavior | 0.493 | 0.029 | 0.437 | 0.550 | 0.437 | 0.551 | <.001 | |||
Subjective norms---Behavior | 0.244 | 0.032 | 0.182 | 0.307 | 0.182 | 0.307 | <.001 | |||
Indirect | ||||||||||
Attitude---Behavior | 0.159 | 0.014 | 0.132 | 0.187 | 0.132 | 0.188 | <.001 | |||
Subjective norms---Behavior | 0.258 | 0.019 | 0.222 | 0.298 | 0.222 | 0.299 | <.001 | |||
Total | ||||||||||
Attitude---Behavior | 0.652 | 0.030 | 0.594 | 0.710 | 0.594 | 0.710 | <.001 | |||
Subjective norms---Behavior | 0.501 | 0.030 | 0.443 | 0.562 | 0.443 | 0.562 | <.001 |
Discussion
Principal Results
Our findings support the hypothesis that intentions mediate vaccination behavior, attitudes, and subjective norms and addresses concern about the mediating process of intention in the theory of planned behavior [
], while also justifying initial theoretical claims that distal attitudes and subjective norms can influence behavior, through proximal intention mediators [ ]. In particular, perceived behavioral control was found to be a moderator that influences the mediating processes attitude→intention→behavior and subjective norms→intention→behavior.Comparison With Prior Work
A large meta-analysis [
] of applications of theory of planned behavior showed that subjective norms were weak predictors of intention and behavior. Recent studies [ , ] that have used the theory of planned behavior empirically for health behavior have suggested that subjective norms are not the most critical predictor. In contrast, some argued that subjective norms are strong predictors [ , ]. Our findings are similar to those in [ , ], and the indirect effects in our study further show that subjective norms have greater impacts on behavior than attitudes. This indicates that decisions about health issues are more likely to be influenced by social surroundings. Debate about whether subjective norms have a strong or weak influence on behavior might due to differences in the types of behaviors that have been targeted. In contrast to high-frequency health behaviors, such as exercise, smoking cessation, when making decisions about vaccination, people expect to be counseled by someone close to them (eg, family members and close friends [ ]), or someone they trust (eg, a physician [ ]). Additionally, socially desirable responses likely contributed to our finding that attitudes have less explanatory power for behavior. Socially desirable responses are defined as the tendency to give a positive self-description, which is relatively common for potentially sensitive problems. Korn [ ] suggested that vaccination is a prosocial behavior and demonstrated that it is part of the social contract by showing that there is significant intergroup bias in vaccinated and unvaccinated cohorts. Therefore, it is reasonable that participants glorified attitudes toward the vaccine and responded positively to the investigation, rather than providing their true thoughts.Our findings also show that high perceived behavioral control weakens the effect between attitude, subjective norms, and vaccination behavior, using intention as a mediator. Additionally, perceived behavioral control has an alternative relationship with attitude and subjective norms, when present as a moderator in the model. These findings extend those in existing literature on perceived behavioral control. However, perceived behavioral control was considered to be a positive predictor of intentions in many existing studies [
- ]. Our findings (block 2 in the hierarchical regression analysis) also confirm this view. It is important to consider that if a given behavior is seen as positive and implementable, people tend to engage in that behavior. However, the gap between intentions and behavior exists precisely because intention is not sufficient to ensure that a person converts ideas into actual behavior, due to the limitations of actual capabilities. Thus, we find that the mechanisms by which perceived behavioral control affect intention and behavior are different. In other words, the influence of perceived behavioral control on intention is as positive as attitude and subjective norms. However, when using perceived behavioral control to predict behavior, it should be thought of as actual behavior control [ ]. Therefore, it is more appropriate to use perceived behavioral control as a moderating variable to influence the mediating effect of intention on behavior.Practical Applications
The moderated mediation model provides evidence for the practical application of theory of planned behavior in vaccination behavior. Future interventions for people who are unwilling to be vaccinated or subjectively postpone vaccination should not only focus on changing their negative attitudes toward the vaccine but should also pay attention to the ideas of their families and close friends about vaccination and intervention. This will facilitate positive intentions to get vaccinated. In a study [
] that explored factory workers' vaccination behavior using the theory of planned behavior, it was also shown that a positive attitude and the support of health care workers, relatives, and friends can contribute to individuals getting vaccinated, and in another study [ ], it was found that trusted individuals had a unique influence on young women in encouraging them to get their HPV vaccination. In addition, health authorities may need to take measures such as communicating risk or giving rewards for vaccinations to improve influenza and HPV vaccine uptake.Limitations
This study was conducted in the form of a web-based survey; thus, convenience sampling might have caused selection bias. Additionally, we used a cross-sectional survey; thus, information on respondents' vaccine-related behavior at the time of the survey was used in behavior dimension. For this reason, the values of composite reliability and average variance extracted for the behavior dimension were below the ideal value. However, the overall model fit was ideal, with GFI, adjusted GFI, and CFI >0.9 and RMSEA <0.06.
Data from 1 month after the survey showed that the actual COVID-19 vaccination rate (having completed at least one shot), in China was 74% [
], approximating the self-reported behavioral data in this study (8614/9924, 86.8%). Although the self-reported data of this study are credible, cautious interpretation of the sample’s representativeness of vaccination behavior is needed.The behavior dimension was measured with 3 items—self-reported COVID-19 vaccination behavior and influenza vaccination behavior, as well as overall vaccine hesitancy. This is because according to the schedule of immunization in China [
], there are 5 main vaccines for adults 18 to 59 years of age—COVID-19, influenza, HPV, hepatitis B, and rabies. Yearly influenza and the recent COVID-19 vaccine were the used to examine vaccination behavior because the administration of the other 3 types are limited—hepatitis B vaccinations are valid for a long period, rabies vaccinations are for emergency use after possible exposure, and HPV vaccinations are mainly target the female population in China.Conclusions
Our findings showed that subjective norms have a stronger influence than attitudes on this particular vaccination practice. Moreover, perceived behavioral control not only is a positive facilitator of intention but also has an alternative relationship with attitudes and subjective norms. As a moderator, perceived behavioral control conversely weakens the positive effects of attitudes and subjective norms on vaccination behaviors. When perceived behavioral control is low, the positive influence of attitudes and subjective norms are evident. However, when perceived behavioral control is high, the positive effects of attitudes and subjective norms gradually decrease. In particular, as a moderator, perceived behavioral control has more predictive power for vaccination behavior.
Acknowledgments
HJ, LY, and SZ designed the study. QW, LY, GY, and TC conducted the literature review and designed the questionnaire. GY, LJ, SZ, and NS assisted with the web-based investigation. LY, QW, and SX analyzed the data. HJ, QW, GY, and LZ interpreted the results. All authors critically revised the manuscript for important intellectual content. This work was supported by the Wuxi City Technology Development Fund (grant N20191007); the Postgraduate Research and Practice Innovation Program of Jiangsu Province (grant KYCX21_0160); and the Public Health Research Center of Jiangnan University (grant JUPH201845). The funders and sponsors did not participate in the work.
Conflicts of Interest
None declared.
Questionnaire.
PDF File (Adobe PDF File), 759 KB
Supplementary material.
PDF File (Adobe PDF File), 578 KBReferences
- WHO Collaborating Centre for Infectious Disease Modelling, MRC Centre for Global Infectious Disease Analysis, Jameel Institute, Imperial College London. Report 43 - quantifying the impact of vaccine hesitancy in prolonging the need for non-pharmaceutical interventions to control the COVID-19 pandemic. Imperial College London. URL: https://www.imperial.ac.uk/mrc-global-infectious-disease-analysis/covid-19/report-43-vaccine-hesitancy/ [accessed 2021-09-19]
- WHO Collaborating Centre for Infectious Disease Modelling, MRC Centre for Global Infectious Disease Analysis, Abdul Latif Jameel Institute for Disease and Emergency Analytics, Business School, Imperial College London, PATH. Report 33 - modelling the allocation and impact of a COVID-19 vaccine. Imperial College London. URL: https://www.imperial.ac.uk/mrc-global-infectious-disease-analysis/covid-19/report-33-vaccine/ [accessed 2021-09-19]
- Loomba S, de Figueiredo A, Piatek SJ, de Graaf K, Larson HJ. Measuring the impact of COVID-19 vaccine misinformation on vaccination intent in the UK and USA. Nat Hum Behav 2021 Mar 05;5(3):337-348. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Glanz K, Bishop DB. The role of behavioral science theory in development and implementation of public health interventions. Annu Rev Public Health 2010;31:399-418. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Mbous YP, Patel J, Kelly KM. A systematic review and meta-analysis of physical activity interventions among colorectal cancer survivors. Transl Behav Med 2020 Oct 12;10(5):1134-1143. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Taylor N, Conner M, Lawton R. The impact of theory on the effectiveness of worksite physical activity interventions: a meta-analysis and meta-regression. Health Psychol Rev 2012 Mar;6(1):33-73. [CrossRef]
- Noar SM, Benac CN, Harris MS. Does tailoring matter? meta-analytic review of tailored print health behavior change interventions. Psychol Bull 2007 Jul;133(4):673-693. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Noar SM, Black HG, Pierce LB. Efficacy of computer technology-based HIV prevention interventions: a meta-analysis. AIDS 2009 Jan 02;23(1):107-115. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Michie S, Carey RN, Johnston M, Rothman AJ, de Bruin M, Kelly MP, et al. From theory-inspired to theory-based interventions: a protocol for developing and testing a methodology for linking behaviour change techniques to theoretical mechanisms of action. Ann Behav Med 2018 May 18;52(6):501-512 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Aimagambetova G, Babi A, Issa T, Issanov A. What factors are associated with attitudes towards HPV vaccination among Kazakhstani women? exploratory analysis of cross-sectional survey data. Vaccines (Basel) 2022 May 23;10(5):824 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Al-Qerem W, Jarab A, Hammad A, Alasmari F, Ling J, Alsajri AH, et al. Iraqi parents' knowledge, attitudes, and practices towards vaccinating their children: a cross-sectional study. Vaccines (Basel) 2022 May 22;10(5):820 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Aljamaan F, Alhaboob A, Saddik B, Bassrawi R, Assiri R, Saeed E, et al. In-person schooling amidst children's COVID-19 vaccination: exploring parental perceptions just after omicron variant announcement. Vaccines (Basel) 2022 May 12;10(5):768 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Corace KM, Srigley JA, Hargadon DP, Yu D, MacDonald TK, Fabrigar LR, et al. Using behavior change frameworks to improve healthcare worker influenza vaccination rates: a systematic review. Vaccine 2016 Jun 14;34(28):3235-3242 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Painter JE, Borba CPC, Hynes M, Mays D, Glanz K. The use of theory in health behavior research from 2000 to 2005: a systematic review. Ann Behav Med 2008 Jun;35(3):358-362. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Armitage CJ, Conner M. Efficacy of the theory of planned behaviour: a meta-analytic review. Br J Soc Psychol 2001 Dec;40(Pt 4):471-499. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Tyson M, Covey J, Rosenthal HES. Theory of planned behavior interventions for reducing heterosexual risk behaviors: a meta-analysis. Health Psychol 2014 Dec;33(12):1454-1467. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Gourlan M, Bernard P, Bortolon C, Romain AJ, Lareyre O, Carayol M, et al. Efficacy of theory-based interventions to promote physical activity. a meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. Health Psychol Rev 2016;10(1):50-66. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Abraham C, Sheeran P. The Health Belief Model. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2007:97-102.
- Carpenter LR, Green AL, Norton DM, Frick R, Tobin-D'Angelo M, Reimann DW, et al. Food worker experiences with and beliefs about working while ill. J Food Prot 2013 Dec;76(12):2146-2154 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Carpenter CJ. A meta-analysis of the effectiveness of health belief model variables in predicting behavior. Health Commun 2010 Dec;25(8):661-669. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Webb TL, Joseph J, Yardley L, Michie S. Using the internet to promote health behavior change: a systematic review and meta-analysis of the impact of theoretical basis, use of behavior change techniques, and mode of delivery on efficacy. J Med Internet Res 2010 Mar 17;12(1):e4 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Lareyre O, Gourlan M, Stoebner-Delbarre A, Cousson-Gélie F. Characteristics and impact of theory of planned behavior interventions on smoking behavior: a systematic review of the literature. Prev Med 2021 Feb;143:106327. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Ajzen I. The theory of planned behaviour: reactions and reflections. Psychol Health 2011 Sep;26(9):1113-1127. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Hair J, Black W, Babin B. Multivariate Data Analysis 6th edition. Upper Saddle River: Pearson Prentice Hall; 2006.
- Fornell C, Larcker DF. Evaluating structural equation models with unobservable variables and measurement error. J Mark Res 1981 Feb;18(1):39. [CrossRef]
- Anderson JC, Gerbing DW. Structural equation modeling in practice: a review and recommended two-step approach. Psychol Bull 1988 May;103(3):411-423. [CrossRef]
- Gerbing DW, Anderson JC. Monte Carlo evaluations of goodness of fit indices for structural equation models. Sociol Methods Res 2016 Jun 29;21(2):132-160. [CrossRef]
- Hu L, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Modeling 1999 Jan;6(1):1-55. [CrossRef]
- Preacher KJ, Rucker DD, Hayes AF. Addressing moderated mediation hypotheses: theory, methods, and prescriptions. Multivariate Behav Res 2007;42(1):185-227. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Cohen J, Cohen P, West SG, Aiken LS. Applied Multiple Regression/Correlation Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Erlbaum: Mahwah; 2003.
- Wolff K. COVID-19 vaccination intentions: the theory of planned behavior, optimistic bias, and anticipated regret. Front Psychol 2021;12:648289 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- La Barbera F, Ajzen I. Control interactions in the theory of planned behavior: rethinking the role of subjective norm. Eur J Psychol 2020 Aug;16(3):401-417 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Liu Y, Yen L, Liaw F, Lin M, Chiang S, Lin F, et al. Gender differences in the extended theory of planned behaviour on smoking cessation intention in young soldiers. Int J Environ Res Public Health 2021 Jul 23;18(15):471-499 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Adams C, Gringart E, Strobel N, Masterman P. Help-seeking for mental health problems among older adults with chronic disease: an application of the theory of planned behaviour. Aust J Psychol 2021 Jul 19;73(4):426-437. [CrossRef]
- Allen JD, Mohllajee AP, Shelton RC, Othus MKD, Fontenot HB, Hanna R. Stage of adoption of the human papillomavirus vaccine among college women. Prev Med 2009 May;48(5):420-425. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Gerend MA, Shepherd JE. Predicting human papillomavirus vaccine uptake in young adult women: comparing the health belief model and theory of planned behavior. Ann Behav Med 2012 Oct;44(2):171-180 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Karafillakis E, Paterson P, Larson HJ. 'My primary purpose is to protect the unborn child': understanding pregnant women's perceptions of maternal vaccination and vaccine trials in Europe. Vaccine 2021 Sep 15;39(39):5673-5679 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Qiu X, Bailey H, Thorne C. Barriers and facilitators associated with vaccine acceptance and uptake among pregnant women in high income countries: a mini-review. Front Immunol 2021;12:626717 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Korn L, Böhm R, Meier NW, Betsch C. Vaccination as a social contract. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2020 Jun 30;117(26):14890-14899 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Xu X, Wang S, Yu Y. Consumer's intention to purchase green furniture: do health consciousness and environmental awareness matter? Sci Total Environ 2020 Mar 20;704:135275. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Buckley L, Kaye S, Pradhan AK. Psychosocial factors associated with intended use of automated vehicles: a simulated driving study. Accid Anal Prev 2018 Jun;115:202-208. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Lee M, You M. Psychological and behavioral responses in South Korea during the early stages of coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19). Int J Environ Res Public Health 2020 Apr 25;17(9):2977 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Zhang KC, Fang Y, Cao H, Chen H, Hu T, Chen Y, et al. Behavioral intention to receive a COVID-19 vaccination among Chinese factory workers: cross-sectional online survey. J Med Internet Res 2021 Mar 09;23(3):e24673 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Krawczyk AL, Perez S, Lau E, Holcroft CA, Amsel R, Knäuper B, et al. Human papillomavirus vaccination intentions and uptake in college women. Health Psychol 2012 Sep;31(5):685-693. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Coronavirus (COVID-19) vaccinations. Our World in Data. URL: https://ourworldindata.org/covid-vaccinations#how-many-covid-19-vaccination-doses-have-been-administered [accessed 2022-02-28]
- Non-immunization planning vaccination program in Guangdong Province (2021 version). Guangdong Provincial Centre for Disease Control and Prevention. URL: http://cdcp.gd.gov.cn/zwgk/jsbzywj/content/post_3437520.html [accessed 2022-05-30]
Abbreviations
CFI: comparative fit index |
GFI: goodness-of-fit index |
HPV: human papillomavirus |
RMSEA: root mean square error of approximation |
Edited by T Sanchez, A Mavragani; submitted 03.11.21; peer-reviewed by B Fordham, YL Chiu; comments to author 02.02.22; revised version received 03.03.22; accepted 26.04.22; published 20.06.22
Copyright©Liuqing Yang, Lili Ji, Qiang Wang, Yan Xu, Guoping Yang, Tingting Cui, Naiyang Shi, Lin Zhu, Shixin Xiu, Hui Jin, Shiqi Zhen. Originally published in JMIR Public Health and Surveillance (https://publichealth.jmir.org), 20.06.2022.
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work, first published in JMIR Public Health and Surveillance, is properly cited. The complete bibliographic information, a link to the original publication on https://publichealth.jmir.org, as well as this copyright and license information must be included.