Original Paper
Abstract
Background: Two psychosocial constructs that have shown consistent associations with negative health outcomes are discrimination and perceived unfairness.
Objective: The current analyses report the effects of discrimination and unfairness on medical, psychological, and behavioral outcomes from a recent cross-sectional survey conducted in a multiethnic sample of adults in Michigan.
Methods: A cross-section survey was collected using multiple approaches: community settings, telephone-listed sample, and online panel. Unfairness was assessed with a single-item previously used in the Whitehall study, and everyday discrimination was assessed with the Williams 9-item scale. Outcomes included mental health symptoms, past-month cigarette use, past-month alcohol use, past-month marijuana use, lifetime pain medication use, and self-reported medical history.
Results: A total of 2238 usable surveys were collected. In bivariate analyses, higher unfairness values were significantly associated with lower educational attainment, lower age, lower household income, and being unmarried. The highest unfairness values were observed for African American and multiracial respondents followed by Middle Eastern or North African participants. Unfairness was significantly related to worse mental health functioning, net adjustment for sociodemographic variables, and everyday discrimination. Unfairness was also related to self-reported history of depression and high blood pressure although, after including everyday discrimination in the model, only the association with depression remained significant. Unfairness was significantly related to 30-day marijuana use, 30-day cigarette use, and lifetime opiate use.
Conclusions: Our findings of a generally harmful effect of perceived unfairness on health are consistent with prior studies. Perceived unfairness may be one of the psychological pathways through which discrimination negatively impacts health. Future studies examining the relationships we observed using longitudinal data and including more objective measures of behavior and health status are needed to confirm and extend our findings.
doi:10.2196/26622
Keywords
Introduction
There has been considerable interest on the part of researchers and policy makers in understanding the social, economic, psychological, and behavioral factors that account for the many health disparities that are evident in the United States and the world [
- ]. Although socioeconomic factors are the most commonly examined drivers of disparities, social and interpersonal factors have also received considerable attention [ , - ]. Two psychosocial constructs that have shown consistent associations with health outcomes are discrimination and perceived unfairness. The former has been extensively examined in both the United States [ , - ] and globally [ , , ], while studies of unfairness and health have been limited to a handful of studies in the United Kingdom and the Netherlands [ - ]. No studies have reported the joint effects of these 2 constructs.In perhaps the first published study on unfairness, DeVogli et al [
] used the prospective data of 8298 individuals from The Whitehall II study, who were civil servants originally ages 35-55 at the study onset in 1985 through 1998. Baseline unfairness data were collected between 1991 and 1993 (during phase 3 of the study), and respondents were tracked for health outcomes on average 11 years through 2003 to 2004 (phase 7 of the study). The authors found that their single-item unfairness measure was associated with higher odds of clinically verified heart disease as well as worse physical and mental functioning, with the latter being assessed with the 36-Item Short Form Survey (SF-36) questionnaire [ , ] adjusted for sociodemographic and other risk factors. Unfairness was also positively related with being female, obese, and less physically active. Higher unfairness was associated with higher cigarette use but also higher rates of alcohol abstinence.In a second study [
], again based on the Whitehall study, phase 3 data on unfairness were used to predict cardiovascular risk factors, over the subsequent 6 years through phase 5. The authors found that baseline unfairness was significantly associated with higher rates of low serum high-density lipoprotein, high serum triglycerides, hypertension, high fasting blood glucose, and elevated waist circumference.A third study, conducted in the Netherlands, used a 9-item perception of unfairness scale, administered in 2008 [
]. Of those completing this unfairness questionnaire, 1282 adults also completed the SF-36 [ , ] in 2003, 2008, and 2010, which was used to classify respondents as experiencing either physical or mental health decline between 2003 and 2010 and between 2008 and 2010, with unfairness scores from 2008 being used as the predictor. In general, higher scores on unfairness were associated with significantly higher odds of both physical and mental health decline. Higher scores on the unfairness measure were also associated with lower socioeconomic position (eg, a composite of income and education).The health effects of discrimination have been reported in hundreds of studies. One measure of discrimination that has been particularly popular is the Everyday Discrimination Scale developed by Williams and others [
, , ]. The Everyday Discrimination Scale focuses on what are sometimes referred to as microaggressions, smaller acts of discrimination, racism, or prejudice; for example, the scale includes items such as “people act as if you are inferior,” “you are treated with less respect than others,” and “people act as if you are dishonest.” Everyday discrimination is distinguished from more major experiences of discrimination, such as being unfairly fired from a job, maltreated by the police, or denied a bank loan.The everyday discrimination scale has been associated with a wide range of adverse mental and physical health outcomes including depression, anxiety, distress [
, , - ], and overall well-being [ , ], breast cancer among women under the age of 50 years [ ], and high blood pressure, although the relationship with the latter is often conditional on other variables [ ]. Some studies have used unfair treatment as the measure of discrimination [ ], although the 2 constructs likely represent different psychological and social phenomena [ ]. One distinction between the 2 constructs is that everyday discrimination measures the occurrence of events, whereas unfairness can be thought of as more related to the perception of such events. Additionally, while discrimination measures multiple potential events, unfairness in the Whitehall studies and in the current investigation was measured with a single global item.No study has reported the relationship of both discrimination and unfairness with either mental or physical health outcomes. The current analyses from a recent cross-sectional survey conducted in a multiethnic sample of adults in the state of Michigan provide insight into the effects of discrimination and unfairness on medical, psychological, and behavioral outcomes.
Methods
Measures
Independent Variables
Unfairness
Unfairness was assessed with a single-item from the Whitehall study [
, ], which was worded as follows: ‘‘I often have the feeling that I am being treated unfairly.’’ Participants rated their response on a 6-point scale (1, strongly disagree; 2, moderately disagree; 3, slightly disagree; 4, slightly agree; 5, moderately agree; and 6, strongly agree). We analyzed unfairness both as a continuous variable and as a categorical variable recoded into 3 levels low (responses 1 and 2), medium (responses 3 and 4), and high (responses 5 and 6) to facilitate data presentation.Discrimination
We used the 9-item Everyday Discrimination scale developed by Williams [
, ]. Respondents indicated how often they experience 9 types of discrimination with responses ranging from “Never” to “Almost Everyday.” We computed a mean across the 9 items, which resulted in a score range of 1-6. Respondents were required to answer at least 5 of the 9 items to be included in the analyses.Dependent Variables
Mental health symptoms were assessed with the Patient Health Questionnaire-4 (PHQ-4) [
] which asks how often over the past 2 weeks participants have experienced the following problems: little interest or pleasure in doing things; feeling down, depressed, or hopeless; feeling nervous, anxious, or on edge; and not being able to stop or control worrying. All items were answered with a 1-4 scale, with 4 being “not at all” and 1 being “nearly every day”. Higher scores are indicative of better mental health status. The Cronbach α for the 4 items in our sample was .91.Health behaviors assessed included current cigarette use, defined as consuming at least 100 cigarettes all time and having smoked on at least some days in the past month [
]; past month alcohol, defined as having consumed at least one drink of any alcoholic beverage at least once in the past 30 days [ ]; past month marijuana, defined as any use in the past 30 days; lifetime pain medication, which was queried with an item from the 2017 Youth Risk Behavior Survey [ ]: “During your life, how many times have you taken prescription pain medicine without a doctor’s prescription or differently than how a doctor told you to use it? (Count drugs such as codeine, Vicodin, OxyContin, Hydrocodone, and Percocet.)” Use was considered more than 2 times in one’s lifetime.Self-reported medical history was assessed by asking if the respondent had ever been diagnosed with cancer; diabetes or high blood sugar; high blood pressure or hypertension; depression; or a heart condition, such as heart attack, angina, or congestive heart failure. Each variable was answered with 0 (no) or 1 (yes).
Demographic variables assessed included age (collapsed into 4 groups: 18-35, 30-45, 45-65, and >65 years), household income (collapsed into 4 groups: under US $10,000, US $10,000 to US $49,999, US $50,000 to US $99,999, and >US $100,000), education (collapsed into 4 groups: high school or lower, some college, college graduate, graduate school or higher), gender, country of birth (United States vs not United States), and marital status (no or yes). These are presented in
.Variable | Respondents, n (%) | |
Gender | ||
Male | 726 (32.50) | |
Female | 1508 (67.50) | |
Education | ||
High school or lower | 558 (25.04) | |
Some college | 690 (30.97) | |
College graduate | 659 (29.58) | |
Graduate school | 321 (14.41) | |
Age (years) | ||
18-35 | 566 (25.76) | |
30-45 | 493 (22.44) | |
45-65 | 881 (40.10) | |
> 65 | 257 (11.70) | |
Income (US$) | ||
Under $10,000 | 227 (10.79) | |
$10,000 to $49,999 | 887 (42.18) | |
$50,000 to $99,999 | 611 (29.05) | |
$>100,000 | 378 (17.97) | |
Born in the United States | ||
Yes | 1902 (85.29) | |
No | 328 (14.71) | |
Race | ||
White | 1183 (53.00) | |
African American | 525 (23.52) | |
Middle Eastern | 403 (18.06) | |
Multiracial | 105 (4.70) | |
Other | 16 (0.72) | |
Marital status | ||
Married/living as married | 1134 (50.90) | |
Not married | 1094 (49.10) | |
Modality | ||
Online | 1415 (63.23) | |
Telephone | 496 (22.16) | |
Paper | 327 (14.61) |
Procedures
Surveys were collected in 2019 using multiple methodologies including community settings, telephone, and online panel. The community sample was a convenience sample, whereas the telephone and online sample was built to match the demographic representation of the University of Michigan Rogel Cancer Center catchment area.
Community Administration for Middle Eastern and North African Participants
The Middle Eastern and North African (MENA) sample was collected at community settings across 3 Michigan counties that included 2 supermarkets frequented by the MENA community, 1 health clinic serving a predominantly Arab American population, 1 health clinic serving a predominantly Chaldean population, a state university with a high number of Arab American students, 4 mosques with a high proportion of Yemeni and Lebanese worshippers, 2 Chaldean churches, and a recreation center frequented by Lebanese youth.
Participants were given the option of completing surveys using pen and paper or online forms (tablet provided), with or without assistance, in English or Arabic. For those opting to complete surveys at home, we provided a self-addressed stamped envelopes or a web address to complete the online version. Both paper and electronic surveys required active consent and testament that the respondent was over 18 and self-identified as Arab or Chaldean. Data collectors, many of whom were fluent in both English and Arabic, were trained in interviewing by study staff. Participants received a US $25 gift card after completing their survey. A total of 406 participants were accrued through this method, 87 of whom completed their survey in Arabic.
Community Administration for White and African American Participants
We distributed surveys at 5 community-based educational events sponsored by the University of Michigan Rogel Cancer Center. Participants were able to complete surveys using printed forms or online with provided tablets, and they received a US $25 gift card after completing their survey. A total of 214 participants were accrued through this method.
Telephone
The telephone survey, conducted by Harris Interactive Inc, used a quota sample to reach 501 adults who indicated that they were aged 18 years or over, living in zip codes serviced by the University of Michigan Rogel Cancer Center, and self-identified as either White/Caucasian or Black/African American. The survey, conducted in English, averaged about 23 minutes and consisted of 44 substantive questions and 18 demographic questions. The survey was fielded from May 1, 20219, to July 9, 2019.
Known landline and cell phone numbers were obtained from Dynata, formerly Survey Sampling International. Numbers were randomly selected from within identified zip codes. We oversampled (with a target of at least 40% of the final sample) African American participants by selecting telephone exchanges that were estimated to have an at least 50% African American population.
A maximum of 8 contacts were attempted for each number: the initial dialing attempt plus up to 7 subsequent dialing attempts. A US $10 incentive was offered to the survey participants. The overall response rate was 9%: 8% for landline numbers and 9% from cell phone numbers. A total of 496 participants were accrued through this method, approximately half through landline and half through cell phone.
Online
A quota sample was recruited via an online panel through a commercial survey research organization (Dynata), which maintains a demographically diverse web panel of people who opt in to taking selected surveys. Panel members who log on to Dynata’s site are routed (in a randomized fashion) to available surveys based on their demographic characteristics and needs of open surveys We provided specific county level quotas for individuals aged 18-80 years in 40 Michigan counties served by the University of Michigan Rogel Cancer Center. We oversampled African American participants so that they would comprise at least 20% of the sample. Additional details about Dynata can be found at their website [
]. A total of 1122 participants were accrued through this method. It is possible that participants could have appeared in both the telephone and online panels; however, given the anonymous nature of the data collection and the fact they were conducted independently, we cannot determine how many might have been duplicated. Based on the number of participants available in the targeted zip codes and the number that completed the surveys, the likelihood is small.Analyses
We first present mean unfairness and discrimination values by sociodemographic variables (
). For mental health symptoms, we present (labeled Model 1) linear regression results for the association of unfairness (trichotomized into low, medium, and high to facilitate data presentation) with mental health symptoms, adjusting for income, education, age, gender, and marital status (this is Model 1 referenced in , , and ). We then present results (labeled Model 2) with discrimination added to the regression model (this is Model 2 referenced in , , and ).For health behaviors and medical history, we first report multivariate logistic regression (ORs and 95% CIs), using unfairness (trichotomized) as the primary independent variable, adjusting for income, education, age, gender, and marital status, and then we report a model adding discrimination (labeled Model 2).
Results
A total of 2238 usable surveys were collected, of which two-thirds (n=1508) were completed by females. A little less than half (980/2228, 43.79%) had college or higher educational attainment, and about half were aged 45 or higher, married, and reported a household income above US $50,000 per year. Most, (1902/2230, 85.29%), were born in the United States. About half (1183/2232, 53.00%) were White, 23.52% (2232/2238) were African American, 18.06% (403/2232) were MENA, and 4.70% (105/2232) were multiracial.
As shown in
, the mean value for the unfairness item was 2.62 (SD 1.61), with a range of 1-6; meanwhile, the mean for everyday discrimination was 1.87 (SD 1.06) with a range of 1-6. The 2 variables were moderately correlated (r=0.54; P<.001).In bivariate analyses, higher unfairness values were significantly associated with lower educational attainment, lower age, lower household income, and being unmarried. Birthplace was unrelated to unfairness. For race, the highest values were observed for African American and multiracial respondents followed by MENA respondents. White respondents reported the lowest values, and they were significantly lower than African American, multiracial, and MENA respondents in pairwise comparisons.
Variable | Unfairness, mean (SD) | Everyday discrimination, mean (SD) | ||
Gender | ||||
Male | 2.62 (1.66) | 1.88 (1.12) | ||
Female | 2.63 (1.59) | 1.87 (1.03) | ||
Educationa | ||||
High school or less | 2.82 (1.67)b,c | 1.97 (1.18)b,c | ||
Some college | 2.76 (1.59)d,e | 1.94 (1.06)d,e | ||
College graduate | 2.42 (1.53)b,d | 1.79 (1.03)b,d | ||
Graduate school | 2.43 (1.63)c,e | 1.72 (0.90)c,e | ||
Agea | ||||
18-35 | 2.94 (1.58)b,c,d | 2.27 (1.18)b,c,d | ||
30-45 | 2.71 (1.61)b,e,f | 2.05 (1.17)b,e,f | ||
45-65 | 2.45 (1.58)c,e | 1.66 (0.89)c,e,g | ||
>65 | 2.31 (1.63)d,f | 1.40 (0.70)d,f,g | ||
Incomea (US $) | ||||
Under 10,000 | 2.97 (1.68)b,c | 2.12 (1.24)b,c,d | ||
10,000 to 49,999 | 2.85 (1.62)d,e | 1.95 (1.07)b,e,f | ||
50,000 to 99,999 | 2.43 (1.51)b,d | 1.81 (0.97)c,e | ||
>100,000 | 2.26 (1.55)c,e | 1.75 (1.08)d,f | ||
Born in the United Statesh | ||||
Yes | 2.62 (1.61) | 1.92 (1.08) | ||
No | 2.68 (1.61) | 1.56 (0.89) | ||
Race | ||||
White | 2.25 (1.48)b,c,d | 1.75 (0.97)b,c | ||
African American | 3.27 (1.67)b,e | 2.12 (1.15)b,d,e | ||
Middle Eastern | 2.73 (1.58)c,e,f | 1.69 (0.97)d,f | ||
Multiracial | 3.20 (1.69)d,f | 2.55 (1.36)c,e,f | ||
Other | 2.75 (1.39) | 2.17 (1.04) | ||
Marital statusa | ||||
Married/living as married | 2.38 (1.56)b | 1.72 (0.97)b | ||
Not married | 2.87 (1.62)b | 2.03 (1.12)b | ||
Total | 2.62 (1.61) | 1.87 (1.06) |
aGroup means differ P<.01.
b-gRows with common superscript differ P<.05
hGroup means in everyday discrimination differ P<.01.
As shown in
, unfairness was significantly related to mental health symptoms, after adjustment for sociodemographic variables (Model 1), with higher unfairness values associated with worse (lower scores) mental health status. In addition to an overall significant effect, all pairwise contrasts were significant. The association, both overall and pairwise, remained significant after inclusion of everyday discrimination scores in the regression model (Model 2). The R2 increased from 0.17 to 0.22 when everyday discrimination was added to the model.Model values | Mental health symptoms, mean (SE) | ||
Model 1b,c | |||
Low unfairness | 13.70 (0.23)d,e | ||
Medium unfairness | 12.45 (0.23)d,f | ||
High unfairness | 10.80 (0.30)e,f | ||
Model 2c,g | |||
Low unfairness | 13.43 (0.22)d,e | ||
Medium unfairness | 12.84 (0.24)d,f | ||
High unfairness | 11.70 (0.30)e,f |
aLower scores indicate worse mental health status.
bModel 1: adjusted for age, gender, race, income, education, and marital status; R2=0.17.
cGroup means differ P<.01.
d-fRows with common superscript differ P<.05.
gModel 2: adjusted for age, gender, race, income, education, marital status, and everyday discrimination; R2=0.22.
As shown in
, unfairness was significantly related to self-reported history of depression and high blood pressure, after adjustment for sociodemographic variables (Model 1). Higher unfairness values were significantly associated with higher odds of depression, when both the middle and high groups were compared to the lowest group, and for high blood pressure, when the highest group was compared to the lowest group. After inclusion of everyday discrimination scores in the model (Model 2), there was still an overall association with depression, and the pairwise contrast of highest to lowest remained significant. In Model 2, the effect of unfairness on high blood pressure disappeared. The addition of everyday discrimination to the model increased the R2 value by 0.01% to 0.02%.Predictor | History of depressionb | History of diabetes | History of heart disease | History of high blood pressure | History of cancer | |||||
Model 1c unfairness (R2) | 0.12 | 0.09 | 0.05 | 0.20 | 0.06 | |||||
Low | REFd | REF | REF | REF | REF | |||||
Middle | 1.57 (1.24-1.98)e | 1.21 (0.92-1.58) | 1.01 (0.69-1.48) | 1.11 (0.87-1.42) | 1.18 (0.82-1.69) | |||||
High | 2.93 (2.18-3.95) | 1.03 (0.72-1.45) | 1.31 (0.84-2.06) | 1.49 (1.08-2.05) | 1.14 (0.71-1.82) | |||||
Model 2f unfairness (R2) | 0.14 | 0.10 | 0.07 | 0.21 | 0.07 | |||||
Low | REF | REF | REF | REF | REF | |||||
Middle | 1.19 (0.92-1.54) | 1.15 (0.82-1.54) | 0.80 (0.52-1.22) | 1.00 (0.761-1.309) | 0.99 (0.67-1.48) | |||||
High | 1.89 (1.35-2.64) | 0.84 (0.57-1.234) | 0.87 (0.52-1.45) | 1.22 (0.85-1.74) | 0.78 (0.46-1.32) |
aUnless otherwise stated, the data are reported as OR (95% CI).
bOverall variable P<.01.
cModel 1: adjusted for age, gender, race, income, education, and marital status.
dREF: reference group.
eItalicized CIs indicate statistically significant ORs.
fModel 2: adjusted for age, gender, race, income, education, marital status, and everyday discrimination.
As shown in
, unfairness was significantly related to 30-day marijuana use, 30-day cigarette use, and lifetime opiate use, after adjustment for sociodemographic variables (Model 1). There was no association with 30-day alcohol use. Specifically, higher unfairness values, when both the middle and highest groups were compared to the lowest group, were associated with significantly higher odds of cigarette and opiate use, whereas for marijuana, the contrast was significant only for the highest compared to the lowest group. When everyday discrimination scores were added to the model (model 2), the effect on marijuana was no longer significant and the effect on cigarettes was only significant for the comparison of the middle to the lowest group. In addition, the effect on alcohol use became significant. However, the effect was in the opposite direction than with the other substances. That is, higher unfairness was significantly associated with lower odds of 30-day alcohol use. The addition of everyday discrimination to the model increased the R2 value by 0.01% to 0.02%.Predictor | 30-day alcohol | 30-day marijuana | 30-day cigarettes | Lifetime opiates | |
Model 1b unfairness (R2) | 0.14 | 0.08 | 0.05 | 0.07 | |
Low | REFc | REF | REF | REF | |
Middle | 0.82 (0.64-1.04) | 1.14 (0.82-1.59)d | 1.69 (1.28-2.23)e,f | 1.37 (1.07-1.74)d | |
High | 0.76 (0.54-1.07) | 1.74 (1.17-2.57)d | 1.96 (1.39-2.76)f | 1.41 (1.03-1.92)d | |
Model 2g unfairness (R2) | 0.14f | 0.09 | 0.08 | 0.08 | |
Low | REF | REF | REF | REF | |
Middle | 0.73 (0.56-0.96)d | 0.89 (0.62-1.28) | 1.38 (1.01-1.87) | 1.00 (0.76-1.30) | |
High | 0.65 (0.45-0.95)d | 1.22 (0.79-1.89) | 1.42 (0.972-2.10) | 0.84 (0.59-1.19) |
aUnless otherwise stated, the data are reported as OR (95% CI).
bModel 1: adjusted for age, gender, race, income, education, and marital status.
cREF: reference group.
dP<.05.
eItalicized CIs indicate statistically significant ORs.
fP<.01.
gModel 2: adjusted for age, gender, race, income, education, marital status, and everyday discrimination.
Discussion
Unfairness was significantly related to worse mental health functioning, net adjustment for sociodemographic variables, and everyday discrimination. Unfairness was related to self-reported history of depression and high blood pressure after an adjustment for sociodemographic variables was made although after including everyday discrimination in the model, only the association with depression remained significant.
Unfairness was also significantly related to 30-day marijuana use, 30-day cigarette use, and lifetime opiate use. Adding everyday discrimination scores to the model attenuated these effects, with the association with marijuana no longer being significant and the effect on cigarettes only being significant for the comparison of the middle group to the lowest group.
Our findings of a generally harmful effect of perceived unfairness on health are consistent with prior studies [
- ]. Perceived unfairness, although moderately correlated with everyday discrimination, appears to serve as a unique predictor of health status and health behavior. Alternatively, given that, in some instances, adding everyday discrimination to the model attenuated the impact of unfairness on outcomes, unfairness may also serve as a partial mediator of discrimination; that is, it may be one of the psychological pathways through which discrimination negatively impacts health: it may be a consequence of discrimination. On the other hand, adding discrimination to the model (except for mental health) generally only increased the R2 value by 1% or 2%, suggesting that unfairness captures most of the same variance in health outcomes as does discrimination.In our study, we found that higher unfairness was associated with lower educational attainment, lower age, lower household income, and being unmarried. The higher rates of unfairness observed for individuals with lower education and income is consistent with both UK [
, ] and Dutch studies [ ]. Age was unrelated to unfairness in the Whitehall study [ ], which differs from our findings. We found no differences by gender, which is inconsistent with the Whitehall study, where women had higher levels of reported unfairness [ ].With regard to race, we found the highest unfairness values were observed among African American and multiracial respondents, followed by MENA respondents. White respondents reported the lowest values, and they were significantly lower than African American, multiracial, and MENA respondents in pairwise comparisons. Prior studies did not report race effects, as their samples were more homogeneous than that of this study. The Whitehall study sample, for example, consisted of 90% White and 5% South Asian participants [
].We found that the effect of unfairness on 30-day alcohol was in the opposite direction to the other substances we examined. That is, higher unfairness was associated with lower odds of 30-day alcohol use. The protective effect of unfairness on alcohol intake was not entirely unexpected. A few previous studies have found that higher levels of unfairness and discrimination were not associated with increased alcohol use [
, , ]. In our study, this relationship was in part confounded by religiosity, as more religious respondents were both more likely to abstain from alcohol and to report higher levels of unfairness (data not shown). Controlling for religion reduced the protective effect of unfairness on alcohol use.The study has several limitations. First, the data were all cross sectional, which limits our ability to determine causation; reverse causality cannot be excluded. For example, higher rates of illness or substance use could drive higher perceptions of unfairness or discrimination, rather than the inverse.
Secondly, unfairness may be caused by everyday discrimination. Experiencing discrimination could lead one to perceive life as unfair. Given the 2 variables were moderately correlated (r=0.54) and that the effects of unfairness on health outcomes generally remained after adjustment for discrimination, it appears that unfairness may function as a unique contributor to health outcomes, above that of discrimination. It should also be noted that while the 9-item everyday discrimination scale assesses various types of discriminatory behaviors, the single-item unfairness measure we used only assesses global perceptions. Perhaps more granular assessment of unfairness, more akin to how discrimination is typically assessed, would yield more robust findings.
Furthermore, illness outcomes were all self-reported, which likely increased the error of these measures. Given this, the analyses might have underestimated the association of unfairness on health, unless they share a common reporting bias, which then would have spuriously inflated their association. Finally, our sample was recruited in Michigan, so the generalizability of our findings to other populations is unclear.
Our findings have implications for both research and practice. With regard to research, our results indicate that unfairness should be included as a potential variable in studies of health and health disparities in addition to the standard set of socioeconomic and social indicators. Unfairness may also be more amenable to intervention than discrimination and other disparity drivers. Because unfairness relates more to the perception or perseveration of events rather than simply their occurrence, cognitive interventions that help individuals cope with these perceptions may merit investigation.
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by the National Cancer Institute (grant no. P30CA046592-29-S4).
Conflicts of Interest
None declared.
References
- Braveman P. What are health disparities and health equity? We need to be clear. Public Health Rep 2014;129 Suppl 2:5-8 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- 2018 National healthcare quality and disparities report. Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality. Rockville, MD; 2020 Apr. URL: https://www.ahrq.gov/research/findings/nhqrdr/nhqdr18/index.html [accessed 2020-02-01]
- Williams DR, Yan Yu, Jackson JS, Anderson NB. Racial differences in physical and mental health: socio-economic status, stress and discrimination. J Health Psychol 1997 Jul;2(3):335-351. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Ahmed AT, Mohammed SA, Williams DR. Indian J Med Res 2007 Oct;126(4):318-327. [Medline]
- Williams DR. Race, socioeconomic status, and health. The added effects of racism and discrimination. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1999;896:173-188. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Williams DR, Collins C. US socioeconomic and racial differences in health: patterns and explanations. Annu. Rev. Sociol 1995 Aug;21(1):349-386. [CrossRef]
- Borrell LN, Kiefe CI, Williams DR, Diez-Roux AV, Gordon-Larsen P. Self-reported health, perceived racial discrimination, and skin color in African Americans in the CARDIA study. Soc Sci Med 2006 Sep;63(6):1415-1427. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Williams DR, Gonzalez HM, Williams S, Mohammed SA, Moomal H, Stein DJ. Perceived discrimination, race and health in South Africa. Soc Sci Med 2008 Aug;67(3):441-452 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Williams DR, Mohammed SA. Discrimination and racial disparities in health: evidence and needed research. J Behav Med 2009 Feb;32(1):20-47 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Williams DR, Neighbors HW, Jackson JS. Racial/ethnic discrimination and health: findings from community studies. Am J Public Health 2003 Feb;93(2):200-208. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Krieger N, Smith K, Naishadham D, Hartman C, Barbeau EM. Experiences of discrimination: validity and reliability of a self-report measure for population health research on racism and health. Soc Sci Med 2005 Oct;61(7):1576-1596. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Kessler RC, Mickelson KD, Williams DR. The prevalence, distribution, and mental health correlates of perceived discrimination in the United States. J Health Soc Behav 1999 Sep;40(3):208-230. [Medline]
- Schulz AJ, Gravlee CC, Williams DR, Israel BA, Mentz G, Rowe Z. Discrimination, symptoms of depression, and self-rated health among african american women in detroit: results from a longitudinal analysis. Am J Public Health 2006 Jul;96(7):1265-1270. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Moomal H, Jackson PB, Stein DJ, Herman A, Myer L, Seedat S, et al. Perceived discrimination and mental health disorders: the South African Stress and Health study. S Afr Med J 2009 May;99(5 Pt 2):383-389. [Medline]
- De Vogli R, Brunner E, Marmot MG. Unfairness and the social gradient of metabolic syndrome in the Whitehall II Study. J Psychosom Res 2007 Oct;63(4):413-419. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- De Vogli R, Ferrie JE, Chandola T, Kivimäki M, Marmot MG. Unfairness and health: evidence from the Whitehall II Study. J Epidemiol Community Health 2007 Jun;61(6):513-518. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Bosma H, Gerritsma A, Klabbers G, van den Akker M. Perceived unfairness and socioeconomic inequalities in functional decline: the Dutch SMILE prospective cohort study. BMC Public Health 2012 Sep 22;12:818 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- McHorney CA, Ware JE, Lu JF, Sherbourne CD. The MOS 36-item Short-Form Health Survey (SF-36): III. Tests of data quality, scaling assumptions, and reliability across diverse patient groups. Med Care 1994 Jan;32(1):40-66. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Ware JE, Sherbourne CD. The MOS 36-item short-form health survey (SF-36). I. Conceptual framework and item selection. Med Care 1992 Jun;30(6):473-483. [Medline]
- Taylor TR, Williams CD, Makambi KH, Mouton C, Harrell JP, Cozier Y, et al. Racial discrimination and breast cancer incidence in US Black women: the Black Women's Health Study. Am J Epidemiol 2007 Jul 01;166(1):46-54. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Emerson E, Milner A, Aitken Z, Krnjacki L, Vaughan C, Llewellyn G, et al. Overt acts of perceived discrimination reported by British working-age adults with and without disability. J Public Health (Oxf) 2021 Apr 12;43(1):e16-e23. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Kirkham R. Asking the wrong question? The difference between unfairness and discrimination. J Public Health (Oxf) 2019 Dec 02:2. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JB, Löwe B. An ultra-brief screening scale for anxiety and depression: the PHQ-4. Psychosomatics 2009;50(6):613-621. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Health Information National Trends Survey (HINTS). National Cancer Institute. 2018. URL: https://hints.cancer.gov/data/survey-instruments.aspx#H5C2 [accessed 2019-01-03]
- Youth Risk Behavior Survey. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC). 2017. URL: https://www.cdc.gov/healthyyouth/data/yrbs/index.htm [accessed 2019-01-03]
- Dynata. URL: www.dynata.com [accessed 2021-04-09]
- Britton A, Shipley M, Marmot M, Hemingway H. Does access to cardiac investigation and treatment contribute to social and ethnic differences in coronary heart disease? Whitehall II prospective cohort study. BMJ 2004 Aug 07;329(7461):318. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Borrell L, Kiefe C, Diez-Roux A, Williams D, Gordon-Larsen P. Racial discrimination, racial/ethnic segregation, and health behaviors in the CARDIA study. Ethn Health 2013;18(3):227-243 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
Abbreviations
MENA: Middle Eastern and North African |
PHQ-4: Patient Health Questionnaire-4 |
SF-36: 36-Item Short Form Survey |
Edited by G Eysenbach; submitted 18.12.20; peer-reviewed by L Marc; comments to author 23.02.21; revised version received 25.02.21; accepted 06.04.21; published 10.05.21
Copyright©Ken Resnicow, Minal Patel, Molly Green, Alyssa Smith, Elizabeth Bacon, Stefanie Goodell, Dylan Kilby, Madiha Tariq, Asraa Alhawli, Nadia Syed, Jennifer Griggs, Matthew Stiffler. Originally published in JMIR Public Health and Surveillance (https://publichealth.jmir.org), 10.05.2021.
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work, first published in JMIR Public Health and Surveillance, is properly cited. The complete bibliographic information, a link to the original publication on https://publichealth.jmir.org, as well as this copyright and license information must be included.